Early Connections for a Lifetime of Language and Literacy Learning
From From Birth For Life
Children’s language and literacy development is a complicated process influenced by their biology and environment. Most follow a typical progression through a series of developmental milestones. However, the age at which they reach these stages varies from child to child depending on gender, language experience, socioeconomic status and to a lesser degree, birth order (Baker & Wigfield, 1999; Burchinal, Peisner-Feinbreg, Pianta, & Howes, 2002; Nicoladis & Genesee, 1997). Despite this variability, an understanding of the various stages can aid ELCC practitioners in monitoring and promoting growth.
Contents |
Infancy: First steps into language
In the pre-linguistic stage of infancy, children acquire
a number of skills that will develop into verbal communication.
From birth, infants are biologically prepared
to pay attention to the sounds of speech and to
process language by breaking it down into phrases,
words and sounds. Parents and practitioners can encourage
this natural ability by speaking often to and
around children. It is common in some cultures – including
European/North American culture – to speak
to babies and young children using a slower rate and
exaggerated pitch and enunciation (e.g. “Are you
mommy’s baby? Yes you are!”). This infant-directed
speech – previously called motherese – can help
foster a newborn’s awareness of language. Through
exposure to infant-directed speech and other models
of language, five-month-olds become sensitive to the
most common sounds in their native language and
they can distinguish among familiar voices (Jusczyk,
2002). By seven and half months, infants can break
down fluent speech into individual words, and at
eight months they generally respond to a few words
representing people (e.g., “mommy,” “daddy” and
the infant’s name). By 11 months, infants understand
10 to 150 words (Fenson et al., 1994).
Throughout the resource kit, when a 'range of skills' is described, it indicates the span between the bottom 10% and the top 10% of children in the reference group.
While infants are learning to understand language, they are also developing the skills needed to produce language. For the first two months of life, they primarily vocalize by crying. At approximately two months, infants begin to coo and a few months later begin to babble. Cooing is characterized by vowel sounds (e.g., “aaaaeeee”); babbling involves consonant vowel combinations (e.g., “dadadada”). At around three months, infants begin to engage in vocal turn taking, and by eight months their pattern of vocalization consistently resembles conversations: they are silent while the caregiver speaks and resume vocalizing when the caregiver pauses to listen (de Boysson-Bardies, 1999). Around 11 months infants may create and use protowords, which are unique combinations of syllables that infants use repeatedly to refer to specific objects (e.g., “baba” to refer to a pacifier) (Robb, Bauer & Tyler, 1994). At this time some children may also be producing between 0 to 20 real words (Fenson et al., 1994).
Infants in the pre-linguistic stage also develop
nonverbal methods of communication, such as joint
attention. During joint attention, the child follows
the caregiver’s gaze toward objects and vice versa.
For example, when a practitioner looks at a ball, the
infant may follow his or her gaze and also focus on
the ball. If the practitioner names the ball, the infant
begins to form connections between language and
the physical world, which rapidly increases their
vocabulary (Campbell & Namy, 2003). Infants also
learn to communicate non-verbally by pointing,
making facial expressions and waving. As with verbal
communication, infants’ use of gestures can vary
widely. For example, eight-month-old infants may
use between 3 and 20 gestures; 14-month-olds may
use from 23 to 52.
TABLE 1 | |
Summary of Language and Literacy Milestones (Infancy) | |
Age |
Milestone |
Newborn |
|
1-2 Months |
|
3-7 Months |
|
8-12 Months |
|
(Fenson, et al., 1994; Boyson-Bardies, 1999) |
Toddlers: Exploring the world of words
Around 12 months and older, children begin to
understand and produce speech to interact with others
and to express their needs and wants. Although
toddlers may develop vocabulary at varying rates,
their language development tends to follow a similar
sequence. Between 12 and 24 months they start to
use holophrastic speech, in which single words represent
a number of different meanings depending on
the context (Barrett, 1982). For example, a toddler
may use the word “sock” to mean “the sock is over
there,” “put the sock on,” or “take it off.” The next
stage is often called telegraphic speech, named for
its similarity to the language typically used in a telegram.
Telegraphic speech contains short, two-word
sentences made up of crucial content words, and the
meaning of these sentences can vary widely depending
on the context (Bloom, Lightbrown, Hood, Bowerman,
Maratsos & Maratsos, 1975). For example,
“mommy go” could mean “Mom is leaving,” “Mom,
I want to leave,” “Mom, I want you to leave,” or “Is
Mommy leaving?” depending on the specific context
in which the phrase is used.
Between the ages of 12 to 24 months, children understand considerably more words than they produce. At 12 months toddlers may understand between 25 and 200 words, yet may produce no words or as many as 25 words (Fenson, et al., 1994). Some children rapidly increase their spoken vocabulary in a short period of time (sometimes called a “vocabulary burst”). Other children develop language slowly over longer periods (Fenson et al., 1994; Goldfield & Resnik, 1990). Over the preschool period, typically developing children learn 17 words on average per week until the age of seven. However, the number of words learned is heavily influenced by their environment. Those children who hear less speech in their home or ELCC setting are generally slower to learn new words, acquiring approximately 11 words on average per week in the early years (Biemiller, 2005). Thus, by 24 months toddlers may produce between 50 and 550 words (Fenson, et al., 1994). Toddlers often make errors when producing new words (de Boysson-Bardies, 1999). They may generalize the meaning of words inappropriately by over- or under-extending the meaning. Overextending the meaning of a word occurs when a toddler uses “Rover” to refer to all dogs, not just his own dog. Conversely, under-extending occurs when the toddler uses “dog” to refer only to their pet and not to other dogs. Once again, language exposure influences the number and type of words learned by an individual child. ELCC practitioners may foster language development by elaborating and using a variety of synonyms (e.g., big, large, gigantic) in everyday contexts (Hoff & Naigles, 2002).
Toddlers demonstrate their growing language awareness by comprehending and acting on words and phrases without external hints. At approximately 13 months, toddlers understand and respond to some spoken instructions, such as “Look at the sleeping dog,” without hints from body language or eye gaze (Thomas, Campos, Shucard, Ramsay & Shucard, 1981). As children grow they become more sensitive to the role of each individual word in a phrase and other aspects of grammar. By 20 months, children use cues from the sentence structure and from the context to extract the meaning of words. For example, when exposed to a new pretend word like “daxy,” children use the surrounding sentence context to determine whether the new word is a proper noun (e.g. “This is Daxy”), or a common noun (e.g., “This is a daxy”) (Bélanger & Hall, 2006).
Nonverbal communication also continues to improve
steadily over the 12 to 36 month period. Toddlers
become experts at joint attention and begin to understand
the meaning of other non-linguistic gestures
(Behne, Carpenter & Tomasello, 2005). In one study,
parents looked and pointed at an uninteresting box.
Fourteen-month-old children followed their parent’s
non-verbal signal and guessed that there was a
reason for calling attention to the box. Based on this guess, the children generally chose to look inside the
box, where they found a prize (Behne et al., 2005).
This demonstrates that children understand that
adults focus on people or objects for a reason, and
toddlers use that information to guide their actions.
TABLE 2 | |
Summary of Language and Literacy Milestones (Toddlers) | |
Age |
Milestone |
12 Months |
|
12-18 Months |
|
18 Months |
|
18-24 Months |
|
24-36 Months |
|
Note: The information presented forms a consensus in the literature as these milestones are frequently cited in overview texts and websites (Boyson-Bardies, 1999; Child Development Institute, 2005; The Canadian Association of Speech-Language Pathologists and Audiologists (CASLPA), 2006; Fenson et al., 1994) |
Preschool (ages 3 to 4): Playing with letters and grammar
Between the ages of three and four, children’s utterances
become increasingly sophisticated and they
begin to produce grammatically correct speech.
They use “-s” to indicate plural, and “-ed” to indicate
past tense. Initially, when children are learning
to use affixes they may over-generalize the use of
these grammatical units. For example, a child might
use the regular rule when it should not be applied,
and say “tooths,” instead of “teeth” or “goed,”
instead of “went.” Over-generalization of grammatical
rules, while technically incorrect, is a positive
sign that children are learning and applying the rules
of grammar. It is usually not effective for adults to
correct these types of errors; they usually self-correct
over time (Marcus, Pinker, Ullman & Hollander,
1992).
At age three, early literacy skills begin to develop
and then continue to progress in parallel with language
skills. Literacy development is determined
heavily by the physical and social environment provided
by parents and ELCC practitioners (Jalongo,
Dragich, Conrad, & Zhang, 2002). Through shared
storybook reading, children learn to hold a book,
turn the pages in order, look at the pages from left to
right, and follow along with the illustrations. During
reading, children may assign basic labels and ask
questions about the visual content of the book. For
example, in response to “Where is the duck?” the
child points to the image, or the child may point
at the duck and ask, “What’s that?” Simple picture
books are particularly useful for fostering these
skills (Jalongo et al., 2002).
The skills children develop are also influenced by direct teaching. For
example, with instruction, three-year-olds can name
the letters of the alphabet and segment the initial
sounds of a word (e.g., /p/ of “pot” or the /m/ of
mommy; Aram & Biron, 2004; Whitehurst & Lonigan,
1998). Children actively taught about letters,
phonological awareness (e.g., ability to recognize
and talk about the sounds of speech) and writing
skills perform better on literacy tasks than older children
who were not provided with this training (Aram
& Biron, 2004).
TABLE 3 | |
Summary of Language and Literacy Milestones (Preschoolers) | |
Age |
Milestone |
3-4 years |
|
Note: the information presented forms a consensus in the literature as these milestones are frequently cited in overview texts and websites (Boyson-Bardies, 1999; Child Development Institute, 2005; The Canadian Association of Speech-Language Pathologists and Audiologists (CASLPA), 2006; Fenson et al., 1994) |
Preschool (ages four to six): Connecting language and literacy
At age four many children participate in organized
activities or lessons, and by age five or six many
children are enrolled in kindergarten or first grade.
During the later preschool years, children’s vocabulary
increases at a rate of 800 to 1000 words per year
(Biemiller & Slonim, 2001). By late in their fifth
year, children can comprehend and produce thousands
of words (Anglin, 1993; Biemiller & Slonim,
2001). Children use their expanding vocabulary to
produce more complex language. Older preschoolers
begin to skilfully use language and grammatical
conventions to form questions (e.g., “What was I
eating?”), negatives (e.g., “I was not eating carrots.”)
and compound sentences (e.g., “I was eating
cheese and it was yummy.”). They also understand
relational contrasts (e.g., big-little, heavy-light) and
use them in sentences (e.g., “My truck is bigger than
yours.”). At age five, children generally understand
and use passive sentence structure (e.g., “The car
was hit by a truck.”) (Shaffer, Wood, & Willoughby,
2002).
At this stage, phonological awareness becomes an increasingly important skill. Around age four, children demonstrate this skill by clapping along with each syllable or sound and by recognizing words that rhyme (e.g. bat and cat) (National Institute of Child Health and Human Development [NICHHD], 2000). Growth in phonological awareness leads to many new skills. For example, children become able to identify which word does not belong in a group using phonological information (e.g., rat, rag, river, bag). They also become able to break words into their parts (e.g., /b/-/ae/-/t/ in bat) (NICHHD, 2000).
Older preschoolers also have a growing understanding
of written language and the conventions of print.
Four-year-olds begin to understand that sentences
are broken into words, words are made of letters
and letters are oriented in a certain way on the page
(Levy, Gong, Hessel, Evans, & Jared, 2006). Many
five-year-olds can handle a picture book, turn the
pages correctly and form a comprehensive narrative
based on the visual images displayed (Jalongo et al.,
2002). Typically, they have good print awareness
skills (e.g., letter orientation), but cannot distinguish
real words (e.g., basket) from strings of consonants
and vowels (e.g., bneaort) or from pretend words
(e.g., bornt) (Levy et al., 2006).
TABLE 4 | |
Summary of Language and Literacy Milestones (School-age) | |
Age |
Milestone |
4-6 years |
|
Note: the information presented forms a consensus in the literature as these milestones are frequently cited in overview texts and websites (Boyson-Bardies, 1999; Child Development Institute, 2005; The Canadian Association of Speech-Language Pathologists and Audiologists (CASLPA), 2006; Fenson et al., 1994) |